Harvard Psychological Studies, Volume 1 - Containing Sixteen Experimental Investigations from the Harvard Psychological Laboratory. by Various
page 45 of 880 (05%)
page 45 of 880 (05%)
![]() | ![]() |
|
at just the rate of the pendulum, it moved _more rapidly_ rather than
more slowly. The image is thus horizontally elongated, by an amount which varies from the least possible up to 9 cm. (the width of the opening in _T_), or _even more_. And while the last of the movement (_O_ to _P_, or _N_ to _P'_), in which the stimulation of _H'_ is supposed to subside, is indeed executed, it may yet be done so _rapidly_ that after all _H'_ cannot subside, not even although it is now less intense by being horizontally spread out (that is, less concentrated than the vanished _h_ of shape 4). This explanation is rendered more probable by the very rare appearance of shape 5, which must certainly emerge if ever the eye were to move more slowly than the pendulum. The critical fact is, however, that shape 4 _does_ appear to a trained subject in about one half the trials--a very satisfactory ratio when one considers the difficulty of timing the beginning of the movement and its rate exactly to the pendulum. Lastly, in some cases no image appears at all. This was at first a source of perplexity, until it was discovered that the image of the dumb-bell, made specially small so as to be contained within the area of distinct vision, could also be contained on the blind-spot. With the pendulum at rest the eye could be so fixed as to see not even the slight halo which diffuses in the eye and seems to lie about the dumb-bell. It may well occur, then, that in a movement the image happens to fall on the blind-spot and not on the fovea. That this accounts for the cases where no image appears, is proved by the fact that if both eyes are used, some image is always seen. A binocular image under normal convergence can of course not fall on both blind-spots. It may be further said that the shape 4 appears as well |
|